×

우리는 LingQ를 개선하기 위해서 쿠키를 사용합니다. 사이트를 방문함으로써 당신은 동의합니다 쿠키 정책.

image

English LingQ Podcast 1.0, One hundred and twenty one: Interview with Paul Nation, one

One hundred and twenty one: Interview with Paul Nation, one

Steve: We're talking today with Paul Nation who is a leading expert on language learning and English language learning, in particular, who's located at the University of New Zealand, I believe. Hi Paul.

Paul: Hi, how are you?

Steve: Fine thank you. Is it, in fact, the University of New Zealand?

Paul: No, it used to be about 100 years ago, but now we're Victoria University of Wellington.

Steve: Okay, I'm sorry.

Paul: Named after Queen Victoria because it was started when she was on the throne.

Steve: Okay.

We last met when I was in Taiwan about two or three years ago and you spoke to an audience of about 100 eager English teachers in Taiwan.

Paul: Oh yeah.

Steve: You explained your four threads and they were very interesting.

Paul: Four strands.

Steve: Four strands, rather, and you were able to refer to your own experience in learning Japanese and I think other languages and so forth. Maybe we could begin by my asking you to explain the four strands.

Paul: Sure. The idea behind the four strands is to make sure that there's a range of opportunities for learning. It's really a way of a teacher or a course designer checking a course to make sure there's a proper range of opportunities for the learners to learn the language. The four strands are these: the first strand is the strand of meaning-focused input, which Steve Krashen would call comprehensible input and that's learning through reading and through listening. There are certain conditions which have to apply for that learning to take place and Stephen Krashen calls it comprehensible input. From a vocabulary perspective it means that only about 1 in 50 of the running words should be unknown to the people who are doing the reading or the listening.

Steve: 1 in 50?

Paul: 1 in 50. We've done an experiment to show that. It would be good to have more research on it; in fact, we've got a Ph.D. student who's going to start doing more research on that, but it actually agrees with a figure that Michael West arrived at almost 80 years ago when he started designing the very first graded readers.

Steve: Right.

Paul: So that's the first strand the strand of learning through input. The second strand is the strand of learning through output. I call it meaning-focused output where the learners are focusing on conveying messages; getting messages across to listeners or getting messages across to readers through writing and through speaking. Having to produce language makes you pay attention to input in a different way, but also provides good opportunity for consolidating knowledge that you've already got.

The third strand is the strand of language-focused learning. Rod Ellis and others call it form-focused instruction, but I'm not so happy with that name because it doesn't actually have to be instruction by a teacher and it can focus on more than form it can focus on meaning as well. I call it language-focused learning and that means deliberate learning of language features through studying the sound system, the spelling system, vocabulary and so on.

I'm actually very excited about a piece of Ph.D. research that one of our students has completed. That student looked to see if you learned vocabulary deliberately on word cards, does this give you the kind of knowledge, the implicit knowledge, which is needed for normal language use. She actually found that this deliberate learning resulted in both explicit knowledge and implicit knowledge, which is different from the learning of grammar because with the learning of grammar there seems to be evidence that there is no direct route into implicit knowledge through the deliberate learning of grammar. It's a rather indirect route, but for vocab it seems different.

Steve: Okay.

Paul: Deliberately-study vocab is available for in-implicit knowledge for normal language use, so that's the third strand.

Steve: Right.

Paul: Then the fourth strand is the strand of fluency development and fluency development simply means getting good at using what you already know. This should be at every level of a language course, so even when you've just learned the numbers in a very elementary course you should learn how to recognize those numbers very quickly, so if someone says 98 you can get 98 quickly and you can understand what they say. The idea with fluency development is you get really quick and fast at using what you've already learned.

Steve: Oh, okay.

Paul: Now three of these four strands, meaning focused-input, focused-output and fluency development, are message-focused strands. They would fit nicely into a communicative approach to language teaching for example. The language-focused learning strand is a deliberate study strand so it's a bit different from the other three, but I think you have to have all four in a course and you have to have roughly equal proportions of the four.

Steve: Alright. You know it's very interesting because we're doing what we're doing with our system and I very much would like to have and I very much appreciate you agreeing to sort of have an interchange with me because I'm not a university professor, I'm someone who has been in business and who has a great interest in languages and we've developed this system called LingQ. But if I listen to the things that you've described to me here, I'll tell you what we do is similar and different. We place a lot of emphasis on input and 1 in 50 unknown words strikes me as very low and I'll explain why. I would agree with you if I were reading a book and I had nothing else to help me. I have been learning Russian using our system starting from scratch and you can't be 1 in 50 because initially it's zero.

Paul: That's right.

Steve: Because what we do is you read something, we ask people to listen to it three-four-five times and to read it. They look up unknown words and the words go to a database. This starts to develop then a database that they can look at in terms of flashcards, it develops statistics, etc.

I listened to call it learner language for a while and read learner language listening 20-30-40 times and then I moved into authentic content and, of course, when I first went into authentic content our system tells me that there's 40 or 50% new words there for me, so that was very hard going, but I did it. Now I'm down to about 15 to 20% new words if I'm reading Tolstoy (????? 7:00) or whatever, but our learners say that they like to be between 10 and 20%. Granted, we're talking about total words, so we're not talking about word families.

People are quite comfortable as long as they're reading on a computer with access to an online dictionary where stuff is going into a database. Our system highlights words they have previously saved so they can refresh on those, so yes input, yes comprehensible. We tend to encourage people to deal with something that's a little more challenging and a lot of our learners say they're happy doing so. I'm not sure if that contradicts what you're saying, but that's sort of what we're doing.

Paul: The way I'd look at is that, first of all, the 98% coverage is for unassisted reading.

Steve: Okay, right.

Paul: That means you're reading without a dictionary and without that help. I guess it's sort of – how would you say – almost intuitive personal observation of when in your reading most of your focus is on language features or most of your focus is on getting the message. If you're doing assisted reading, as you suggest, that assisted reading could be meaning-focused input as long as the major focus is on the message of the text.

Steve: Let's put it this way, the way we do it, what we try to suggest and the way I'm motivated is I'm interested in the story. If we can get our learners, again, to select content that they like, which in my case is 19th century literature, which might be a bit of an esoteric interest you know, I don't mind going at it with 20% unknown words because I'm interested in what I'm doing. As I listen to it for the third and fourth time and I read it for the second and third time and then I go through all my flashcards the whole experience is enjoyable for me.

Paul: You could argue that what you're doing is really covering three strands of the course. When you start off with your reading you're clearly doing language-focused learning because there is so much that is unknown.

Steve: Right.

Paul: We've done studies on English and to get 98% coverage of English -- where you're naturally including proper nouns as words -- that can be considered as known, so they are part of that 98% coverage. You actually need 9,000 words or for a novel you'd need about 8,000 words to read a novel with 98% coverage.

Steve: Now you're talking word families.

Paul: Yes.

Steve: Okay.

Paul: I'm talking 8 to 9,000 word families to get 98% coverage of what we call the running words or tokens.

Steve: But I've read some of your material and you suggest a 1 to 1.6 ratio…

Paul: …no, no, no.

Steve: …of word families to total word count. I read that in something you wrote somewhere.

Paul: Yeah, that's old.

Steve: Oh, is that old?

Paul: Even though one of the first principles that students have to follow when they enter my course is that you respect age that's one that I wouldn't fight for now.

Steve: I'll stop quoting you then.

Paul: The problem with that figure is it's dependent on the length of the text because you're looking at a variant of type-token ratio and type-token ratio is very strongly dependent on text length.

Steve: Oh, I see.

Paul: If you have a really long text then that ratio of actual words to the number of times it's repeated then can go up.

Steve: I'm not talking about number of times it's repeated, I'm saying in English where you have “go” and “going”.

Paul: You're saying how many members in a word family.

Steve: Yeah, because it just so happens that in our system we consider every different form of the word as a different word. What we do in our system is when you save a word we automatically capture all phrases in all of your content that use this word and “going” is used differently than “go”. You gather a bunch of phrases where the word “going” is used, so you get credit for “go”, “going” and “when”; they're all different.

Paul: Yes.

Steve: I understood you in one of your articles to say that the difference between a word…like if you say 9,000 word families, I would say that on our count you need 1,500 words.

Paul: Word types.

Steve: Word types.

Paul: Yeah, okay. Well, there's a quick and easy way to find this out.

Steve: Right.

Paul: One way is to go to my website and download the program called The Range Program. There's a version there based on the British National Corpus, which goes up to the first 14,000 word families of English. If you run that program and look at the figures just below the table it will tell you how many families are in each list, which is 1,000, but it will also tell you how many types there are in the same list. There's a very, very big difference in the number of types amongst the high-frequency families compared to the lower-frequency families.

Steve: No doubt; far more types in the high-frequency ones.

Paul: Yeah, so if you look at the first 1,000 – I can't remember the figures – there'd be probably five or seven or eight members of a family; whereas if you look at say the 8 or 9,000 there's barely two members to a family.

Steve: For sure, yeah, okay.

Paul: So you can't keep a standard figure for that because it differs so slightly from one frequency level to the other. The other point I'd raise about that is you say that each word is used differently.

Steve: Right.

Paul: When you look at production, (...) speaking and writing, then I think the word type is probably the best unit for counting. But if you're looking at reception that is understanding through reading and listening, I think the word family is a better unit for counting.

Steve: Okay.

But one of the things we're trying to do in our system is get people, eventually, to be able to use them. When they save a word in the form of say “going” they will collect 10 sample phrases of that word in use, which they're supposed to practice. Eventually they'll show up when they go to write, so they'll find “going” is used differently than “go”. We don't necessarily treat it as purely receptive or purely productive.

Paul: That's fair enough.

Steve: I don't think I disagree with what you're saying.

Paul: No, I don't think we're disagreeing with each other.

Steve: No.

Paul: John Sinclair would agree with you in terms of output because he argues that even different inflections, you know the “ing” or the pluraless, results in a word having different collocates.

Steve: Exactly and that's what you're looking for is which words are used with which words. I find in Russian where there's a tremendous amount of inflection, I just go about and I save all the different forms of these words. Our words show up in our vocabulary area sorted in different ways, but if I look at them alphabetically or if I search by roots I'll find 7-8-10-15 words that are all very similar and then I can just look at them and get a sense of which ones are used how kind of thing. We like saving individual types, as you call them.

Paul: I think you're also doing a good thing there. At the moment, we've got a Ph.D. student just starting who's looking at if you know the first 1,000 words of English how many lower frequency words does this give you access to. Say you come across “astonish”, “astonish” actually has the root of “stone”, so when you're astonished you turn to stone.

Steve: Right.

Paul: Now maybe that doesn't help with astonish very much, but what he's looking at is to see that if you actually know the first 1,000 what roots in that will help you with words from the lower-frequency levels. No one's actually quantified that in a very systematic way.

Steve: But you know what, based on my experience with Russian I would look at it quite differently.

Learn languages from TV shows, movies, news, articles and more! Try LingQ for FREE

One hundred and twenty one: Interview with Paul Nation, one One|one hundred|||||||| Einhunderteinundzwanzig: Interview mit Paul Nation, ein Ciento veintiuno: Entrevista con Paul Nation, uno Cent vingt et un : Entretien avec Paul Nation, un Centoventuno: Intervista a Paul Nation, uno 百二十一ポール・ネーションへのインタビュー Sto dwadzieścia jeden: Wywiad z Paulem Nationem, jeden Cento e vinte e um: Entrevista com Paul Nation, um Сто двадцать один: Интервью с Полом Нейшеном, 1 Yüz yirmi bir: Paul Nation ile röportaj, bir 第一百二十一:保罗·内申访谈,一 第一百二十一:保羅‧內申訪談,一

Steve: We’re talking today with Paul Nation who is a leading expert on language learning and English language learning, in particular, who’s located at the University of New Zealand, I believe. Hi Paul.

Paul: Hi, how are you?

Steve: Fine thank you. Is it, in fact, the University of New Zealand?

Paul: No, it used to be about 100 years ago, but now we’re Victoria University of Wellington. |||||||||||||||Wellington Paul: No, hace unos 100 años, pero ahora somos la Universidad Victoria de Wellington. Paul: Hayır, yaklaşık 100 yıl önceydi ama şimdi Victoria University of Wellington'ız.

Steve: Okay, I’m sorry.

Paul: Named after Queen Victoria because it was started when she was on the throne. |||||||||during her reign|Queen Victoria||||

Steve: Okay.

We last met when I was in Taiwan about two or three years ago and you spoke to an audience of about 100 eager English teachers in Taiwan. ||||||||||||||||||||||enthusiastic|||| ||||||||||||||||||||||ansiosos|||| ||||||||||||||||||||||entusiastas|||| La última vez que nos vimos fue hace dos o tres años, cuando estuve en Taiwán y usted habló ante un centenar de profesores de inglés de Taiwán.

Paul: Oh yeah.

Steve: You explained your four threads and they were very interesting. Steve speaking|||||主题||||| |||||hilos||||| Steve: Has explicado tus cuatro hilos y han sido muy interesantes.

Paul: Four strands. ||elements ||fios ||cuatro hebras Paul: Cuatro hebras.

Steve: Four strands, rather, and you were able to refer to your own experience in learning Japanese and I think other languages and so forth. ||方面|||||||||||||||||||||| ||linhas|||||||||||||||||||||| Maybe we could begin by my asking you to explain the four strands. ||||||||||||fios Quizá podríamos empezar pidiéndole que me explique los cuatro hilos. Может быть, мы начнем с того, что я попрошу вас объяснить, что такое четыре нити.

Paul: Sure. The idea behind the four strands is to make sure that there’s a range of opportunities for learning. It’s really a way of a teacher or a course designer checking a course to make sure there’s a proper range of opportunities for the learners to learn the language. ||||||||||designer||||||||||||||||||| The four strands are these: the first strand is the strand of meaning-focused input, which Steve Krashen would call comprehensible input and that’s learning through reading and through listening. |||||||componente|||||||||||||||||||||| There are certain conditions which have to apply for that learning to take place and Stephen Krashen calls it comprehensible input. Bu öğrenmenin gerçekleşmesi için geçerli olması gereken bazı koşullar vardır ve Stephen Krashen buna anlaşılabilir girdi adını vermektedir. From a vocabulary perspective it means that only about 1 in 50 of the running words should be unknown to the people who are doing the reading or the listening.

Steve: 1 in 50?

Paul: 1 in 50. We’ve done an experiment to show that. It would be good to have more research on it; in fact, we’ve got a Ph.D. student who’s going to start doing more research on that, but it actually agrees with a figure that Michael West arrived at almost 80 years ago when he started designing the very first graded readers. ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||graduados| estudiante que va a empezar a investigar más sobre eso, pero en realidad coincide con una cifra a la que llegó Michael West hace casi 80 años, cuando empezó a diseñar los primeros lectores graduados. Bu konuda daha fazla araştırma yapmaya başlayacak olan öğrenci, aslında Michael West'in neredeyse 80 yıl önce ilk dereceli okuyucuları tasarlamaya başladığında ulaştığı bir rakamla uyuşuyor.

Steve: Right.

Paul: So that’s the first strand the strand of learning through input. The second strand is the strand of learning through output. La segunda vertiente es la del aprendizaje a través de los resultados. I call it meaning-focused output where the learners are focusing on conveying messages; getting messages across to listeners or getting messages across to readers through writing and through speaking. ||||||||||||transmitindo||||||||||||||||| ||||||||||||transmitir||||||||||||||||| Ben buna öğrencilerin mesaj iletmeye odaklandığı anlam odaklı çıktı diyorum; dinleyicilere mesaj iletmek ya da yazma ve konuşma yoluyla okuyuculara mesaj iletmek. Having to produce language makes you pay attention to input in a different way, but also provides good opportunity for consolidating knowledge that you’ve already got. ||||||||||||||||||||巩固知识||||| Tener que producir la lengua hace que prestes atención a lo que se te dice de una manera diferente, pero también es una buena oportunidad para consolidar los conocimientos que ya tienes. Dil üretmek zorunda olmak, girdiye farklı bir şekilde dikkat etmenizi sağlar, ancak aynı zamanda zaten sahip olduğunuz bilgileri pekiştirmek için iyi bir fırsat sunar.

The third strand is the strand of language-focused learning. Üçüncü kol ise dil odaklı öğrenme koludur. Rod Ellis and others call it form-focused instruction, but I’m not so happy with that name because it doesn’t actually have to be instruction by a teacher and it can focus on more than form it can focus on meaning as well. Rod Ellis ve diğerleri buna biçim odaklı öğretim diyor, ancak ben bu isimden pek memnun değilim çünkü aslında bir öğretmen tarafından verilen bir öğretim olması gerekmiyor ve biçimden daha fazlasına odaklanabilir, anlama da odaklanabilir. I call it language-focused learning and that means deliberate learning of language features through studying the sound system, the spelling system, vocabulary and so on. |||||||||intentional||||||||||||||||

I’m actually very excited about a piece of Ph.D. research that one of our students has completed. That student looked to see if you learned vocabulary deliberately on word cards, does this give you the kind of knowledge, the implicit knowledge, which is needed for normal language use. ||||||||||||||||||||||隐性|||||||| ||||||||||||||||||||||implícito|||||||| Bu öğrenci, kelime kartlarından kasıtlı olarak kelime öğrenip öğrenmediğinizi, bunun size normal dil kullanımı için gerekli olan bilgi türünü, örtük bilgiyi verip vermediğini görmeye çalıştı. She actually found that this deliberate learning resulted in both explicit knowledge and implicit knowledge, which is different from the learning of grammar because with the learning of grammar there seems to be evidence that there is no direct route into implicit knowledge through the deliberate learning of grammar. |||||||led to||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||| De hecho, descubrió que este aprendizaje deliberado daba lugar tanto a un conocimiento explícito como a un conocimiento implícito, lo cual es diferente del aprendizaje de la gramática, ya que con el aprendizaje de la gramática parece haber pruebas de que no hay una ruta directa hacia el conocimiento implícito a través del aprendizaje deliberado de la gramática. Она обнаружила, что такое целенаправленное обучение приводит как к эксплицитному, так и к имплицитному знанию, что отличается от изучения грамматики, поскольку при изучении грамматики, как представляется, есть доказательства того, что нет прямого пути к имплицитному знанию через целенаправленное изучение грамматики. Aslında bu kasıtlı öğrenmenin hem açık bilgi hem de örtük bilgi ile sonuçlandığını bulmuştur ki bu dilbilgisi öğreniminden farklıdır çünkü dilbilgisi öğreniminde, dilbilgisinin kasıtlı öğrenimi yoluyla örtük bilgiye doğrudan bir yol olmadığına dair kanıtlar var gibi görünmektedir. It’s a rather indirect route, but for vocab it seems different. Es una vía bastante indirecta, pero para el vocabulario parece diferente.

Steve: Okay.

Paul: Deliberately-study vocab is available for in-implicit knowledge for normal language use, so that’s the third strand.

Steve: Right.

Paul: Then the fourth strand is the strand of fluency development and fluency development simply means getting good at using what you already know. This should be at every level of a language course, so even when you’ve just learned the numbers in a very elementary course you should learn how to recognize those numbers very quickly, so if someone says 98 you can get 98 quickly and you can understand what they say. The idea with fluency development is you get really quick and fast at using what you’ve already learned.

Steve: Oh, okay.

Paul: Now three of these four strands, meaning focused-input, focused-output and fluency development, are message-focused strands. They would fit nicely into a communicative approach to language teaching for example. The language-focused learning strand is a deliberate study strand so it’s a bit different from the other three, but I think you have to have all four in a course and you have to have roughly equal proportions of the four. Dil odaklı öğrenme dizisi bilinçli bir çalışma dizisidir, bu nedenle diğer üçünden biraz farklıdır, ancak bir kursta dördüne de sahip olmanız gerektiğini ve dördünün kabaca eşit oranlarda olması gerektiğini düşünüyorum.

Steve: Alright. You know it’s very interesting because we’re doing what we’re doing with our system and I very much would like to have and I very much appreciate you agreeing to sort of have an interchange with me because I’m not a university professor, I’m someone who has been in business and who has a great interest in languages and we’ve developed this system called LingQ. ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||同意|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||| ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||intercambio de ideas|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||| But if I listen to the things that you’ve described to me here, I’ll tell you what we do is similar and different. We place a lot of emphasis on input and 1 in 50 unknown words strikes me as very low and I’ll explain why. I would agree with you if I were reading a book and I had nothing else to help me. Я бы согласился с Вами, если бы я читал книгу и мне больше ничего не помогало. I have been learning Russian using our system starting from scratch and you can’t be 1 in 50 because initially it’s zero.

Paul: That’s right.

Steve: Because what we do is you read something, we ask people to listen to it three-four-five times and to read it. They look up unknown words and the words go to a database. This starts to develop then a database that they can look at in terms of flashcards, it develops statistics, etc. ||||then||||||||||||||| Bu, daha sonra bilgi kartları açısından bakabilecekleri bir veri tabanı geliştirmeye başlar, istatistikler vb. geliştirir.

I listened to call it learner language for a while and read learner language listening 20-30-40 times and then I moved into authentic content and, of course, when I first went into authentic content our system tells me that there’s 40 or 50% new words there for me, so that was very hard going, but I did it. Bir süre öğrenen dili olarak adlandırdığımız dili dinledim ve 20-30-40 kez öğrenen dili dinlemesi okudum ve sonra özgün içeriğe geçtim ve tabii ki özgün içeriğe ilk girdiğimde sistemimiz bana orada benim için %40 veya 50 yeni kelime olduğunu söyledi, bu yüzden bu çok zordu ama başardım. Now I’m down to about 15 to 20% new words if I’m reading Tolstoy (????? Ahora sólo leo entre un 15 y un 20% de palabras nuevas si leo a Tolstoi (????? 7:00) or whatever, but our learners say that they like to be between 10 and 20%. Granted, we’re talking about total words, so we’re not talking about word families. Concedido|||||||||||| De acuerdo, estamos hablando de palabras totales, así que no hablamos de familias de palabras.

People are quite comfortable as long as they’re reading on a computer with access to an online dictionary where stuff is going into a database. Our system highlights words they have previously saved so they can refresh on those, so yes input, yes comprehensible. ||resalta|||||||||||||||| We tend to encourage people to deal with something that’s a little more challenging and a lot of our learners say they’re happy doing so. Tendemos a animar a la gente a que se enfrente a algo un poco más difícil y muchos de nuestros alumnos dicen que están contentos haciéndolo. I’m not sure if that contradicts what you’re saying, but that’s sort of what we’re doing. |||||contradicts||||||||||

Paul: The way I’d look at is that, first of all, the 98% coverage is for unassisted reading. |||||||||||||||without assistance| |||||||||||||||sin ayuda| ||||||||||||cobertura|||sem assistência|

Steve: Okay, right.

Paul: That means you’re reading without a dictionary and without that help. I guess it’s sort of – how would you say – almost intuitive personal observation of when in your reading most of your focus is on language features or most of your focus is on getting the message. If you’re doing assisted reading, as you suggest, that assisted reading could be meaning-focused input as long as the major focus is on the message of the text. |||assisted||||||assisted|||||||||||||||||||

Steve: Let’s put it this way, the way we do it, what we try to suggest and the way I’m motivated is I’m interested in the story. If we can get our learners, again, to select content that they like, which in my case is 19th century literature, which might be a bit of an esoteric interest you know, I don’t mind going at it with 20% unknown words because I’m interested in what I’m doing. ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||specialized or niche||||||||||||||||||| ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||esotérico||||||||||||||||||| As I listen to it for the third and fourth time and I read it for the second and third time and then I go through all my flashcards the whole experience is enjoyable for me.

Paul: You could argue that what you’re doing is really covering three strands of the course. ||||||||||abrangendo||||| When you start off with your reading you’re clearly doing language-focused learning because there is so much that is unknown.

Steve: Right.

Paul: We’ve done studies on English and to get 98% coverage of English -- where you’re naturally including proper nouns as words -- that can be considered as known, so they are part of that 98% coverage. ||||||||||||||||correctly named|||||||||||||||| ||||||||||||||||próprios|||||||||||||||| You actually need 9,000 words or for a novel you’d need about 8,000 words to read a novel with 98% coverage.

Steve: Now you’re talking word families.

Paul: Yes.

Steve: Okay.

Paul: I’m talking 8 to 9,000 word families to get 98% coverage of what we call the running words or tokens. |||||||||||||||||palavras Пол: Я говорю о 8-9 тысячах семейств слов, чтобы получить 98% охвата того, что мы называем бегущими словами или лексемами. Paul: Çalışan sözcükler veya belirteçler dediğimiz şeyin %98'ini kapsamak için 8 ila 9.000 sözcük ailesinden bahsediyorum.

Steve: But I’ve read some of your material and you suggest a 1 to 1.6 ratio…

Paul: …no, no, no.

Steve: …of word families to total word count. ...de familias de palabras al número total de palabras. I read that in something you wrote somewhere.

Paul: Yeah, that’s old.

Steve: Oh, is that old?

Paul: Even though one of the first principles that students have to follow when they enter my course is that you respect age that’s one that I wouldn’t fight for now. Пол: Несмотря на то, что один из первых принципов, которому должны следовать студенты, приходя на мой курс, - это уважение к возрасту, я бы не стал бороться за него сейчас. Paul: Öğrencilerin kursuma girdiklerinde uymaları gereken ilk ilkelerden biri yaşa saygı duymak olsa da, bu şu anda uğruna savaşmayacağım bir ilke.

Steve: I’ll stop quoting you then. Steve: Dejaré de citarte entonces.

Paul: The problem with that figure is it’s dependent on the length of the text because you’re looking at a variant of type-token ratio and type-token ratio is very strongly dependent on text length. |||||cifra|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||| Paul: El problema con esa cifra es que depende de la longitud del texto, porque estás viendo una variante de la relación tipo-token y la relación tipo-token depende mucho de la longitud del texto. Paul: Bu rakamla ilgili sorun, metnin uzunluğuna bağlı olmasıdır çünkü tip-token oranının bir çeşidine bakıyorsunuz ve tip-token oranı metin uzunluğuna çok güçlü bir şekilde bağlıdır.

Steve: Oh, I see.

Paul: If you have a really long text then that ratio of actual words to the number of times it’s repeated then can go up. Paul: Si el texto es muy largo, puede aumentar la proporción de palabras reales con respecto al número de veces que se repite. Paul: Eğer gerçekten uzun bir metniniz varsa, o zaman gerçek kelimelerin tekrarlanma sayısına oranı artabilir.

Steve: I’m not talking about number of times it’s repeated, I’m saying in English where you have “go” and “going”.

Paul: You’re saying how many members in a word family.

Steve: Yeah, because it just so happens that in our system we consider every different form of the word as a different word. Steve: Sí, porque resulta que en nuestro sistema consideramos cada forma diferente de la palabra como una palabra diferente. What we do in our system is when you save a word we automatically capture all phrases in all of your content that use this word and “going” is used differently than “go”. You gather a bunch of phrases where the word “going” is used, so you get credit for “go”, “going” and “when”; they’re all different. |reúne||||||||||||||||||||||

Paul: Yes.

Steve: I understood you in one of your articles to say that the difference between a word…like if you say 9,000 word families, I would say that on our count you need 1,500 words. Steve: Bir makalenizde bir kelime arasındaki farkın... örneğin 9.000 kelimelik aileler diyorsanız, bizim hesabımıza göre 1.500 kelimeye ihtiyacınız olduğunu söyleyebilirim.

Paul: Word types.

Steve: Word types.

Paul: Yeah, okay. Well, there’s a quick and easy way to find this out. Hay una forma rápida y sencilla de averiguarlo.

Steve: Right.

Paul: One way is to go to my website and download the program called The Range Program. There’s a version there based on the British National Corpus, which goes up to the first 14,000 word families of English. If you run that program and look at the figures just below the table it will tell you how many families are in each list, which is 1,000, but it will also tell you how many types there are in the same list. There’s a very, very big difference in the number of types amongst the high-frequency families compared to the lower-frequency families.

Steve: No doubt; far more types in the high-frequency ones.

Paul: Yeah, so if you look at the first 1,000 – I can’t remember the figures – there’d be probably five or seven or eight members of a family; whereas if you look at say the 8 or 9,000 there’s barely two members to a family. |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||quase||||| |||||||||||||cifras||||||||||||||||||||||apenas||||| Paul: Evet, yani ilk 1.000'e bakarsanız - rakamları hatırlayamıyorum - muhtemelen bir ailenin beş veya yedi veya sekiz üyesi olurdu; oysa 8 veya 9.000'e bakarsanız bir ailenin ancak iki üyesi vardır.

Steve: For sure, yeah, okay.

Paul: So you can’t keep a standard figure for that because it differs so slightly from one frequency level to the other. ||||||||||||difere||||||||| Paul: Así que no se puede mantener una cifra estándar para eso porque difiere muy ligeramente de un nivel de frecuencia a otro. Paul: Yani bunun için standart bir rakam tutamazsınız çünkü bir frekans seviyesinden diğerine çok az farklılık gösterir. The other point I’d raise about that is you say that each word is used differently. ||||levantar|||||||||||

Steve: Right.

Paul: When you look at production, (...) speaking and writing, then I think the word type is probably the best unit for counting. But if you’re looking at reception that is understanding through reading and listening, I think the word family is a better unit for counting. Pero si lo que se busca es una recepción que comprenda a través de la lectura y la escucha, creo que la familia de palabras es una unidad mejor para contar. Ancak okuma ve dinleme yoluyla anlamaya yönelik bir alımlama söz konusuysa, kelime ailesinin sayım için daha iyi bir birim olduğunu düşünüyorum.

Steve: Okay.

But one of the things we’re trying to do in our system is get people, eventually, to be able to use them. When they save a word in the form of say “going” they will collect 10 sample phrases of that word in use, which they’re supposed to practice. Eventually they’ll show up when they go to write, so they’ll find “going” is used differently than “go”. We don’t necessarily treat it as purely receptive or purely productive. |||||||接收||| |||||||receptiva|||

Paul: That’s fair enough. Paul: Me parece justo.

Steve: I don’t think I disagree with what you’re saying.

Paul: No, I don’t think we’re disagreeing with each other.

Steve: No.

Paul: John Sinclair would agree with you in terms of output because he argues that even different inflections, you know the “ing” or the pluraless, results in a word having different collocates. ||||||||||||||||||||||||no plural|||||||associated words |||||||||||||sostiene||||inflecciones lingüísticas|||||||sin plural|||||||colocaciones diferentes Paul: John Sinclair estaría de acuerdo con usted en términos de producción, ya que sostiene que incluso diferentes inflexiones, ya sabes, el "ing" o el plural, resulta en una palabra que tiene diferentes colocaciones.

Steve: Exactly and that’s what you’re looking for is which words are used with which words. I find in Russian where there’s a tremendous amount of inflection, I just go about and I save all the different forms of these words. ||||||||||词形变化|||||||||||||| ||||||||||flexión gramatical|||||||||||||| Our words show up in our vocabulary area sorted in different ways, but if I look at them alphabetically or if I search by roots I’ll find 7-8-10-15 words that are all very similar and then I can just look at them and get a sense of which ones are used how kind of thing. ||||||||ordenadas||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||| Nuestras palabras aparecen en nuestra área de vocabulario clasificadas de diferentes maneras, pero si las miro alfabéticamente o si busco por raíces encontraré 7-8-10-15 palabras que son todas muy parecidas y entonces puedo mirarlas y hacerme una idea de cuáles se usan y cómo. We like saving individual types, as you call them.

Paul: I think you’re also doing a good thing there. At the moment, we’ve got a Ph.D. student just starting who’s looking at if you know the first 1,000 words of English how many lower frequency words does this give you access to. estudiante que acaba de empezar que está mirando si sabe las primeras 1.000 palabras del inglés a cuántas palabras de menor frecuencia le da esto acceso. Say you come across “astonish”, “astonish” actually has the root of “stone”, so when you’re astonished you turn to stone. |||||atordoado|||||||||||||| ||||Asombrar|||||||||||asombrado|||| Digamos que te encuentras con "asombrar", "asombrar" en realidad tiene la raíz de "piedra", así que cuando te asombras te conviertes en piedra.

Steve: Right.

Paul: Now maybe that doesn’t help with astonish very much, but what he’s looking at is to see that if you actually know the first 1,000 what roots in that will help you with words from the lower-frequency levels. Paul: Ahora puede que eso no ayude mucho con astonish, pero lo que está mirando es ver que si realmente conoces las primeras 1.000 qué raíces en eso te ayudará con palabras de los niveles de menor frecuencia. Paul: Şimdi bu belki astonish konusunda çok yardımcı olmayabilir, ancak baktığı şey, ilk 1.000 kökü gerçekten biliyorsanız, bunun size daha düşük frekans seviyelerindeki kelimelerde yardımcı olacağını görmektir. No one’s actually quantified that in a very systematic way. |||measured|||||系统化| |||cuantificado|||||| Aslında hiç kimse bunu sistematik bir şekilde ölçmemiştir.

Steve: But you know what, based on my experience with Russian I would look at it quite differently.